18 AUGUST 2007, Page 17

Don't knock paranoia. It may be terrifying — but it could save your life

MATTHEW PARRIS Acouple of years ago, trying a freefall parachute jump for the first time and experiencing a new way of hurtling through space, I also discovered that I was a potential paranoiac.

This did not come entirely as a surprise. As a graduate student at Yale I experimented with LSD. Why anyone ever thought this drug would sweep the world and reduce the youth of the West to a state of gibbering addiction I cannot imagine because it was no fun at all, just weird. Among a number of temporary alterations to my perception there were two of a paranoid nature: walking the streets of New Haven, Connecticut, I kept hearing, in the indistinct conversations of strangers, my own name. Realising this was probably the result of eating two little pieces of blotting paper, I kept my nerve, told myself the perception was unreal, and waited for it to fade.

The second mind alteration, though, never entirely did fade. On closing my eyes, the shapes and patches that often swim across our darkened vision seemed to resolve themselves (as clouds can) into faces and figures — grotesque, frightening images. I can still do this by concentrating hard; and a bad experience some years ago with the anti-malarial drug Lariam sharpened and magnified the effect. One of my brothers, who has never taken mind-altering drugs, says this happens to him anyway, and perhaps it is no more than the gestalt process by which the human mind, confronted by apparently random and meaningless visual data, may impose or 'find' meaningful shapes and patterns. The meaning is not inconsistent with the data but nor is it necessarily implied by it. The point in my brother's and my case is that the meaning we found was threatening. Joan of Arc would probably have seen angels.

I think all paranoia may be a form of gestalt. Take the free-fall parachute experience. I had not expected to be scared but, alone (apart from the pilot) with the instructor to whom I was to be strapped, and as our little plane spiralled up slowly to 10,000 feet, it seemed to dawn on me that this was a conspiracy to kill me. I watched him like a hawk as he did up all the buckles which attached us, desperately fighting the fear (like all those years ago in New Haven) and telling myself I was not thinking straight and my conspiracy theory was ridiculous. Why would an Arizonan called Zak who knew nothing about me and had never met me before, plan to fake an accident in which his human attaché became detached and tumbled to his death? Ah — but maybe he did know about me and had been put up to it by someone. Now, who might actually desire my death? Well, there was . . . and away my imagination galloped again.

I managed to rein it in, and managed the jump without incident, but on landing almost vomited with the psychological tension. Since then I have never regarded paranoiacs as quite as sick or as other as once they seemed.

How do we explain paranoia? Obviously chemicals, fear or fatigue are capable of triggering the mental process but the ability to use gestalt in this way must be incipient in all of us. By what Darwinian process has the human animal become hard-wired to read danger into signals that may not imply danger at all? This, I submit, is a survival mechanism, a precautionary piece of mental circuitry designed to err on the side of overreaction because the consequences of undue suspicion are less life-threatening than to miss a set of sinister clues, which can be fatal. 'Paranoia' is simply an inflamed version of a sensitivity which can be life-saving. Stare hard enough at that bush and you see the outlines of the lurking tiger. You might be paranoid, but then again there might be a tiger.

Are many forms of mental illness, similarly, a case of overstimulated survival mechanisms? I wondered about this a few weeks ago, reading reports of research into memory block: a mental process which can cause us genuinely to forget events whose recollection threatens to inflict more pain than we can handle. Selective amnesia could be called a mental illness yet could be a saving force. Psychoanalysts' attempts to make patients recover and 'deal with' buried horrors from the past may be forcing the override of a health-giving piece of brain circuitry.

But what about depression? Or about the ti-polar' or manic-depressive tendency present to some degree in all of us, to become by turns 'irrationally' elated, positive and optimistic, and then unreasonably depressed, passive and pessimistic? Could these too, when experienced to a degree which seriously disables a person, be no more than the hyperstimulation of what are, at a more moderate level, life-enhancing responses?

It is easier to see how the manic 'high' which some experience could be useful within a tribe, if not always to the individual. This — this irrational sense of invulnerability — is what is needed in battle, and can be observed too in fighting-dogs and horses. Kay Jamieson, who writes about and has suffered from bi-polar disorder, says that at high times the bi-polar individual (Churchill, perhaps?) can achieve great things A rush of confidence and adrenaline touches most of us at times, and the experience is positive, as all athletes know.

But how about the lows? We lack useful estimates of the extent of 'clinical' depression in modern Britain, statistics being warped both by the complicity of the medical profession in the drug-peddling tactics of modern drug-company marketing, and by welfare benefit regulations which inflate the diagnosis of depression, which has become the new back pain. Nevertheless serious, bona-fide depression is surely not uncommon, and can be real, crushing and terrifying.

It is also disabling. But could mild depression confer any Darwinian advantage? An associated clutch of mental states such as anxiety, insecurity, worry and stress (even a tendency to take a gloomy view of prospects generally) could be gathered under the term 'precaution' and might, I suppose, be seen as a useful arrow in the quiver of any group of animals, though not if all suffer it simultaneously. An excess of blithe optimists is dangerous, and a tribe is the stronger for containing a good few Eeyores, Jeremiahs and Doubting Thomases, even if this makes our own lives less fun. There might be evolutionary reasons for these genes recurring strongly and regularly among our race and being present to some degree in all of us.

So depression may be a form of gestalt too: a way of reading significance into data, and interpreting the world. I am beginning to think that the dual (and linked) roles of gestalt and what I call 'screening' — an animal's ability to screen out and screen in, from an undigested mess of information, what it must focus upon and what it must elbow aside in order to make that focus — are absolutely central to the evolution of a functioning intelligence.

Matthew Pan-is is a columnist for the Times